Eating disorder
Summarize the articla below 150 or more words
http://informahealthcare.com/jmh
ISSN: 0963-8237 (print), 1360-0567 (electronic)
J Ment Health, 2014; 23(2): 51–54
! 2014 Shadowfax Publishing and Informa UK Limited. DOI: 10.3109/09638237.2014.889286
EDITORIAL
Eating disorders ‘‘mental health literacy’’: an introduction
Jonathan M. Mond 1,2
1 Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia and 2 Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University,
Canberra, Australia
Introduction
No doubt there are many things that readers of this journal
would not agree upon. What constitutes ‘‘evidence-based
treatment’’, how best to revise classification schemes, and the
priority given to prevention, early intervention and treatment
approaches to mental health improvement, are some examples.
One thing that I hope all readers can agree upon,
however, is that community knowledge and understanding
of mental health problems is not as good as it should be.
Community knowledge and understanding of eatingdisordered
behavior may be particularly poor. Reasons for
this likely include the fact that certain eating disorders, such
as binge eating disorder, are relatively new to the psychiatric
nomenclature and the fact that certain eating disorder
behaviors, such as extreme dietary restriction and excessive
exercise, have strongly ego-syntonic properties . The goal of
this contribution is to introduce readers to a field of research
that I believe has promise in redressing this situation, namely,
‘‘mental health literacy’’ . After outlining the origins of the
mental health literacy paradigm, I shall do my best to explain
why its application to eating-disordered behavior has merit
and how research in this field might be progressed.
The mental health literacy paradigm
Jorm et al. (1997) introduced the term ‘‘mental health
literacy’’ (MHL), in the mid-1990s, to refer to ‘‘knowledge
and beliefs about mental disorders which aid their recognition,
management or prevention’’(p.182). The rationale was
that improving community awareness and understanding of
the nature and treatment of mental health problems was not, at
this time, a priority for government health agencies. As a
consequence, members of the public were unsure of the
symptoms of different mental health problems and of how to
respond to, or prevent, the occurrence of those symptoms in
themselves or others. This situation contrasted with that for
physical health problems, where it was accepted that the
public would benefit by knowing what actions they could take
to prevent disease, how to recognize warning signs and assist
others in the event of emergencies, and the likely benefits of
available treatments (Jorm, 2012).
The research methodology could not be simpler – a vignette
of a fictional person suffering from a given mental health
problems is presented and questions about that problem are
posed to study participants. This is the MHL paradigm.
Aspects of MHL examined by Jorm (2012) and others thus far
include: beliefs about the nature and causes of, and risk factors
for, mental health problems; recognition of the symptoms that
constitute a mental disorder; knowledge of and beliefs about
treatment options and their availability; attitudes and beliefs
that may be conducive to stigma and discrimination; and
knowledge and understanding of how to assist others who may
be developing or experiencing a mental disorder.
The use of large, general population surveys in MHL
research has permitted stratification of the data by participants’
demographic characteristics and symptom levels (Jorm,
2012; Jorm et al., 2000). Demographic differences in MHL
are important because they indicate specific targets for health
promotion efforts, for example, a need to improve MHL
relating to depression among young people in rural and
remote communities. MHL differences between individuals
with and without symptoms, on the other hand, might inform
early intervention efforts. Improving community MHL should
also facilitate early intervention efforts on the part of family
members, friends and others who share information with
and interact with symptomatic individuals (Jorm, 2012;
Jorm et al., 2000).
As I am sure Jorm and colleagues would acknowledge, the
concept of MHL was neither radical nor new. Researchers had
recognized the need to study the knowledge and beliefs of the
public concerning mental health problems for decades
(Hayward & Bright, 1997). The early literature included
studies of knowledge and beliefs about eating disorders
(Branch & Eurman, 1980). What was novel, however, was the
rationale provided for the systematic investigation of knowledge
and beliefs concerning mental health problems, particularly
the view that poor MHL may be a major factor in
low or inappropriate help-seeking among individuals with
symptoms (Andrews et al., 2000; Meltzer et al., 2000). It is a
testament to the efforts of Jorm and colleagues that governments
in many countries now incorporate the assessment of
Correspondence: Jonathan M. Mond, PhD, MPH, Department of
Psychology, C3A 411, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109,
Australia. E-mail: [email protected]
MHL in their mental health plans and use this information to
inform their health promotion agendas.
What is known about ‘‘eating disorders mental
health literacy’’?
Whereas much has been learned about MHL relating to the
‘‘more common mental disorders’’, and to schizophrenia,
‘‘eating disorders mental health literacy’’ (ED-MHL) has not,
thus far, been a priority for researchers or policy makers.
It has therefore not been systematically investigated in the
same way as other mental health problems and the detailed
information required to inform health promotion and early
intervention programs is lacking. Further, it is difficult to
determine what is known because there exists a disparate,
but substantial, body of research that has examined
ED-MHL-related knowledge and beliefs but which has
employed an alternative methodology and/or not used the
term ‘‘mental health literacy’’ (Crisp et al., 2000; Davidson &
Connery, 2003). A systematic review of all relevant research
would be beneficial.
Nevertheless, perusal of the recent literature suggests a
small number of key ‘‘problem areas’’ (Mond et al., 2006b,
2008, 2010a). First, it is apparent that awareness and
understanding of the spectrum of disordered eating that
occurs at the population level is poor (Mond et al., 2006b). To
give just one example, ‘‘eating disorders’’ may be associated,
in the public mind, with anorexia nervosa and the purging
form of bulimia nervosa, whereas binge eating disorder and
the non-purging form of bulimia nervosa may tend to be seen
as ‘‘normative’’ (Gratwick-Sarll et al., 2013). Second, there
appears to be a pervasive belief that eating disorders are either
serious but uncommon or common but trivial when the reality
is that they are both serious and common (Mond et al., 2006a;
Palmer, 2003). Third, attitudes and beliefs likely to be
conducive to stigma, such as the beliefs that individuals with
eating disorders only have themselves to blame and that these
individuals are vain, self-obsessed or weak, are not uncommon
(Crisp et al., 2000; Mond et al., 2006b). In addition,
ED-MHL has been found to vary as a function of individuals’
demographic characteristics and symptom levels. Thus, young
men may consider eating disorders to be less serious than
do young women (Mond & Arrighi, 2011) and individuals
with eating disorder symptoms may be particularly likely to
believe that eating-disordered behavior is acceptable or even
desirable (Mond et al., 2010a).
In terms of whose ED-MHL might be most worthy of
attention, research addressing attitudes and beliefs likely to
be conducive to low or inappropriate help-seeking among
men with disordered eating would be especially welcome,
for several reasons (Mond et al., 2013b). First, men may be
particularly unlikely to seek advice or treatment for an
eating problem. Second, the prevalence of disordered eating
and its impact on quality of life are increasing in men.
Third, much of the existing ED-MHL research has been
confined to the ‘‘high-risk’’ populations of adolescent and
young adult women. Moreover, research addressing the
ED-MHL of men is important because their knowledge,
beliefs and behaviors influence the knowledge, beliefs and
behaviors of the individuals with whom they interact,
including adolescent and young adult women (Mond et al.,
2010a, 2013b).
Efforts will also be needed to identify attitudes and beliefs
on the part of primary care practitioners and other nonspecialist
treatment providers that may undermine effective
care delivery. For example, there is good evidence that
primary care practitioners are diffident in their ability to
recognize and/or screen for the presence of eating disorder
psychopathology (Linville et al., 2012; Mond et al., 2010b).
Primary care practitioners may also be unsure as to the
comparative benefits of different possible treatment
approaches and/or treatment providers and, in turn, appropriate
referral of their patients. However, the issue of what
constitutes ‘‘evidence-based treatment’’ is relevant for both
primary care and specialist treatment providers and is not
straightforward (Mond, 2012).
Perhaps most importantly, efforts will be needed to change
the way that eating disorders are viewed by researchers in
other fields of academia and by those who are in a position to
influence public knowledge, beliefs and policy more generally.
The author’s experience, in Australia and the USA, is
that eating-disordered behavior is not taken seriously as a
public health problem, or, worse still, viewed with contempt,
in public health research and policy circles. Certainly this
would help to explain why eating disorders research is so
rarely featured in leading public health journals (Austin,
2012).
If institutionalized stigma towards eating disorders
research and clinical practice exists, then there is a need to
identify the source of this and do something about it. For
example, if there is a lingering perception that eating
disorders are associated with affluence and privilege and,
therefore, not worthy of public policy attention, then this
misconception needs to be dispelled (Striegel-Moore &
Franko, 2003). The misconception that eating disorders are
either serious but uncommon or common but trivial also
seems to be stubbornly resistant to change, perhaps because
this perception is reinforced by adherence to a dichotomous,
medical-model approach to classification and treatment
(Mond et al., 2009). Recent changes to the DSM diagnostic
criteria for eating disorders, including less stringent criteria
for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa and the inclusion of
binge eating disorder as a formal diagnosis, should go some
way to redressing this problem (Mond, 2013).
Eating disorders mental health literacy and the
‘‘obesity epidemic’’
As I have argued elsewhere (Mond et al., 2009, 2013a), the
way in which body dissatisfaction and disordered eating are
conceptualized in obesity prevention research, namely, as
variables that may need to be assessed as secondary outcomes
– as opposed to variables worthy of attention in their own
right – is particularly unfortunate, given the conspicuous links
between body weight, body dissatisfaction, eating-disordered
behavior and mental health. Body-weight-centric obesity
prevention messages should be of concern to all those with
an interest in the reciprocal relations between physical health
and mental health and efforts to improve ED-MHL need to be
accompanied by efforts to inculcate a more balanced view of
52 J. M. Mond J Ment Health, 2014; 23(2): 51–54
the ‘‘obesity epidemic’’ (Bacon & Aphramor, 2011;
Campos et al., 2006). In the author’s view, the latter would
entail information to the effect that adverse physical and
psychosocial consequences are far more likely to occur for
moderate and severe obesity than for overweight and mild
obesity, that the prevalence of moderate and severe obesity is
relatively low, and that moderate degrees of overweight may
in fact be associated with better physical and mental health
outcomes (Mond et al., 2009).
Why is improving ED-MHL important?
A potential criticism of the MHL paradigm is that changing
knowledge and beliefs does not necessarily lead to behavior
change (Stice et al., 2000). Thus, findings from the first
generation of ED prevention research were seen to be
‘‘disappointing’’ because change in knowledge and beliefs
about eating disorders was associated with little or no
change in eating-disordered behavior (Stice et al., 2000).
However, this argument misses the point (Cowen, 1998;
Mond et al., 2013b). The focus of efforts to improve EDMHL
is on reducing the individual and community health
burden of eating-disordered behavior by reducing stigma and
otherwise changing public knowledge and beliefs in ways
that promote the importance of early, appropriate helpseeking
where this is needed (Cowen, 1998; Mond et al.,
2010a, 2013b). Improving ED-MHL may also serve to
prevent the occurrence of eating-disordered behavior among
individuals at risk or potentially at risk. But that is not the
primary objective.
Of course, the relative merits of different possible
approaches to reducing the health burden of eating-disordered
behavior and other mental health problems – health promotion/
universal prevention, selective prevention and indicated
prevention/early intervention – warrant careful consideration
(Munoz et al., 1996). But it also needs to be remembered that
these different approaches are not mutually exclusive (Mond
et al., 2013b). Efforts to improve ED-MHL at the population
level would complement the current focus of eating disorders
prevention research on selective interventions in high-risk
populations (Stice et al., 2013) and would potentially have
multiple benefits, including: (i) greater willingness to seek
treatment among individuals with symptoms, (ii) improved
uptake of empirically supported treatments, (iii) improved
willingness and ability to intervene on the part of family and
friends, (iv) improved detection and management of eating disordered
behavior in primary care and (v) reduced stigma
associated with eating-disordered behavior and mental health
problems more generally
Needs help with similar assignment?
We are available 24x7 to deliver the best services and assignment ready within 3-12 hours? PAY FOR YOUR FIRST ORDER AFTER COMPLETION..
Get Answer Over WhatsApp Order Paper Now